|
on Neuroeconomics |
Issue of 2019‒11‒04
three papers chosen by |
By: | Jonathan Schulz (George Mason University); Uwe Sunde (University of Munich); Petra Thiemann (Lund University); Christian Thoeni (University of Lausanne) |
Keywords: | selection, laboratory experiments |
Date: | 2019–09 |
URL: | http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:not:notcdx:2019-09&r=all |
By: | Francesco Cerigioni |
Abstract: | Evidence from the cognitive sciences suggests that some choices are conscious and reflect individual volition while others tend to be automatic, being driven by analogies with past experiences. Under these circumstances, standard economic modeling might not always be applicable because not all choices are the result of individual tastes. We propose a behavioral model that can be used in standard economic analysis that formalizes the way in which conscious and automatic choices arise by presenting a decision maker comprised of two selves. One self compares past decision problems with the one the decision maker faces and, when the problems are similar enough, it replicates past behavior (Automatic choices). Otherwise, a second self is activated and preferences are maximized (Conscious choices). We then present a novel method capable of identifying a set of conscious choices from observed behavior and discuss its usefulness as a framework for studying asymmetric pricing and empirical puzzles in different settings. |
Keywords: | Dual processes, similarity, revealed preferences, fluency, automatic choice |
JEL: | D01 D03 D60 |
Date: | 2019–10 |
URL: | http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:upf:upfgen:1673&r=all |
By: | Daniel S. Hamermesh; Rachel A. Gordon; Robert Crosnoe |
Abstract: | We use data from the 11 waves of the U.S. Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development 1991-2005, following children from ages 6 months through 15 years. Observers rated videos of them, obtaining measures of looks at each age. Given their family income, parents’ education, race/ethnicity and gender, being better-looking raised subsequent changes in measurements of objective learning outcomes. The gains imply a long-run impact on cognitive achievement of about 0.04 standard deviations per standard deviation of differences in looks. Similar estimates on changes in reading and arithmetic scores at ages 7, 11 and 16 in the U.K. National Child Development Survey 1958 cohort show larger effects. The extra gains persist when instrumenting children’s looks by their mother’s, and do not work through teachers’ differential treatment of better-looking children, any relation between looks and a child’s behavior, his/her victimization by bullies or self-confidence. Results from both data sets show that a substantial part of the economic returns to beauty result indirectly from its effects on educational attainment. A person whose looks are one standard deviation above average attains 0.4 years more schooling than an otherwise identical average-looking individual. |
JEL: | I24 I26 J71 |
Date: | 2019–10 |
URL: | http://d.repec.org/n?u=RePEc:nbr:nberwo:26412&r=all |